The Role of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies, Cognitive Flexibility, and Distress Tolerance in Death Anxiety Among Women with Hypertension: A Descriptive Correlation Study

authors:

avatar Mehrdad Ghanbarpoor Ganjari ORCID 1 , * , avatar Zahra Khanmohammadzadeh 2 , avatar Hanieh Nobakht ORCID 3 , avatar Habib Eslami Kenarsari ORCID 4

M.A, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Sistan and Baluchestan University, Zahedan, Iran
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Sistan and Baluchestan University, Zahedan, Iran
Department of Psychology, Zahedan Branch, Islamic Azad University, Zahedan, Iran
Clinical Research Development Unit of Poursina Hospital, Guilan University of Medical Sciences, Rasht, Iran

how to cite: Ghanbarpoor Ganjari M, Khanmohammadzadeh Z , Nobakht H, Eslami Kenarsari H. The Role of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies, Cognitive Flexibility, and Distress Tolerance in Death Anxiety Among Women with Hypertension: A Descriptive Correlation Study. Jundishapur J Chronic Dis Care. 2020;9(4):e104117. https://doi.org/10.5812/jjcdc.104117.

Abstract

Background:

Death anxiety and its consequence are among the most important mental health issues that should be considered in hypertension patients. Some studies reported that cognitive emotion regulation strategies, cognitive flexibility, and distress tolerance, both influence and predict mental health.

Objectives:

This study aimed to investigate the role of these variables on death anxiety among women with hypertension.

Methods:

This is a descriptive correlational study that was conducted on 150 women with hypertension who were referred to hospitals in Rasht in 2018. Participants were selected using the convenience sampling method. Death Anxiety scale (DAS), the Distress Tolerance scale (DTS), the cognitive flexibility inventory (CFI), and the short version of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation questionnaire (CERQ) were used to collect the data. Data were analyzed using both descriptive (means and standard deviations) and inferential (the Pearson correlation coefficient and stepwise regression analysis) analyzes using SPSS.

Results:

Overall cognitive emotion regulation strategies (r = -0.20), refocus on planning (r = -0.28), acceptance (r = -0.35), positive reappraisal (r = -0.20), and putting into perspective (r = -0.42) had diverse significant relationships with death anxiety. In contrast, catastrophizing (r = 0.19), rumination (r = 0.19), and self-blame (r = 0.16) had direct significant correlations. Besides, overall cognitive flexibility (r = 0.61), controllability (r = -0.21), alternatives (r = -0.44), behavioral justification (r = -0.23), overall distress tolerance (r = -0.21), tolerance (r = -0.18), appraisal (r = -0.15), and regulation (r = -0.17) had diverse significant correlations with death anxiety; however, absorption (r = 0.52) had a direct significant relationship with death (P ≤ 0.05). The results of the stepwise regression analysis indicated that the research variables were could explain 71% of the variance in death anxiety (R2 = 0.71), and overall cognitive flexibility had the strongest role in explaining death anxiety (beta = -0.67), which was significant at the 1% level (P ≤ 0.0001).

Conclusions:

Based on the findings, cognitive emotion regulation strategies, cognitive flexibility, and distress tolerance were associated with death anxiety, and cognitive flexibility had the strongest role in predicting death anxiety.

1. Background

Mental health problems are major risk factors for both morbidity and mortality, so that, for example, nearly one-third of the US adult population suffers from mental health disorders (1), and it is the third leading cause of death in the world. Based on the World Health Organization (WHO) report, the prevalence of hypertension was 33.44 per 100,000 population in Iran, ranking 67th in the world. Moreover, based on the national reports, 17.5% of Iranians aged 15 to 64 were suffering from hypertension in this year (2).

Death anxiety is a common issue in all societies that negatively affects our intellectual security (3). Fear of death is an emotional feeling that stimulates psychological stress (4). As an inevitable, unknown biological reality, death causes severe stress, particularly among elders (5). Hence, due to our instinct thinking about death is unavoidable. However, suffering from a chronic disease or spending the last days of life bring us back to the idea of death, which in turn causes more fear (6).

Cognitive emotion regulation (CER) is a psychological problem that causes death anxiety. CER strategies contain a set of conscious and unconscious cognitive and behavioral strategies that can reduce, maintain, or raise a particular emotion. People use various emotional strategies to cope with emotional experiences (7). Domaradzka and Fajkowska (8) reported that adaptive CER strategies were inversely associated with anxiety and depression. Moreover, the authors found a direct association between maladaptive CER strategies and anxiety and depression (8).

Another study conducted on diabetic patients reported that positive CER strategies had a diverse and significant relationship with anxiety, while negative CER strategies were negatively associated with anxiety (9). A study reported that mindfulness and CER strategies have important roles in decreasing stress among people with hypertension, which indicates the significance of attention to these factors in treating such patients (10). Cognitive flexibility is another factor that contributes to death anxiety. Psychological flexibility means communicating the present moment in order to adapt/persist on behaviors in the service of chosen values (11), which requires interacting with the present moment and the ability to distinguish oneself from inner thoughts and experiences (12). Flexible people are always curious about the inner and outer world and do not afraid of confronting new experiences (13). The abilities to understand difficult situations as manageable, substitute multiple justifications for life events, and create alternatives to hard situations are features of cognitive flexibility that allow the person to face emotional and social challenges properly (14). Studies (15) have shown that more cognitively flexibility individuals can better cope with the disease and adapt to pain, as well. For example, Safari Mousavi et al. (16) reported that cognitive flexibility and adaptive and maladaptive CER strategies were significantly correlated with depression, anxiety, and stress. In other words, those who are more cognitively flexible and use efficient emotion regulation strategies are optimistic about their problems, accept problems (instead of eliminating), respond appropriately to their surroundings, and move toward their values (16). Distress tolerance is defined as a meta-emotional construct that includes a person’s ability to tolerate, evaluate, and accept negative emotional experiences (17). As distress tolerance is associated with depression and anxiety, it can be examined by symptoms of these disorders. The most common symptoms include insomnia, headache, and fatigue, which can disrupt a person’s daily life (18). Moreover, people with low distress tolerance react more strongly to stress and anxiety compared to others (19). Several studies have emphasized the role of distress tolerance in the emergence of mental disorders (20). Low levels of distress tolerance are associated with increased risk for substance abuse disorders, medicine abuse, binge eating symptoms, and post-traumatic stress symptoms (21). Another study found that distress tolerance is associated with social anxiety disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder (22). Onyedibe et al. (23) showed that distress tolerance is a significant predictor of hypertension. Although, they found that the level of distress tolerance is inversely associated with the risk of hypertension emergence.

Based on the literature, CER, cognitive flexibility, and distress tolerance play important roles in anxiety disorders. Although this association is investigated by several studies, according to the best knowledge of the authors, no study has examined the impact of these variables on death anxiety yet.

2. Objectives

Hypertension has a high prevalence in the general population of Iran. Since women are at increased risk of dying from hypertension (in Iran, 60% of hypertension-related deaths occur in women (24)) and due to their critical role in society, the current study aimed to assess the relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies, cognitive flexibility, and distress tolerance and death anxiety among women with hypertension.

3. Methods

3.1. Research Methods, Statistical Population, and Sample

This is a descriptive correlational study that was conducted on women with hypertension admitted to the hospitals in the city of Rasht in 2018. In total, 150 women were selected using the convenience sampling method. The sample size was calculated using the method used by Gall et al. (25), which selected 15 participants per each variable. The current study had four variables; therefore, the sample size 60 subjects was sufficient. Additionally, according to the formula proposed by Tabachnick et al. (26), i.e., n = 50 + 8. M, 82 subjects were sufficient for the present study. However, the sample size was increased to account for the attrition or loss to follow up. After admitting to the hospital, the objectives of the study were explaining to the eligible patients, and then if they were agreeing, patients were asked to fill the questionnaires. The authors did their best to follow the Declaration of Helsinki on ethical principles (27). Ethical considerations were respected, including patients’ confidentiality, obtaining informed consent, explaining participants about objectives of the study, respecting patients’ willingness to drop out any time, and confidentiality of data. Inclusion criteria were having a systolic hypertension above 140 and diastolic hypertension above 90, being female, being aged 30 to 60 years, having at least elementary school education, and having no background disease. Exclusion criteria were suffering from pain and discomfort, not willing to participate in the study, and having mental disorders (according to the subject’s statements or medications).

3.2. Questionnaires

3.2.1. The Death Anxiety Scale

This scale was designed by Templer in 1970. The DAS consists of 15 yes (1) - no (0) statements that should be answered using a five-point Likert scale with the anchors 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 5 (“strongly agree”) with a high score of 8, which indicates a high level of anxiety. Its construct validity was confirmed using the factor analysis method. To assess the reliability of the Death Anxiety scale (DAS), the test-retest method was used, which produced a correlation coefficient of 0.83 (28). Gholami et al. (29) reported a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.81 for this scale.

3.2.2. The Distress Tolerance Scale

The Distress Tolerance scale (DTS) (developed by Simmons and Gaher) is a 15-item scale composed of 4 subscales, namely: tolerance (1, 3, and 5), absorption (2, 4, and 15), appraisal (6, 7, 9, 10, and 12), and regulation (8, 13, and 14). Its scoring system is based on a five-point Likert-type scale, anchors 1 (“strongly agree”) to 5 (“strongly disagree”). One item (item number six) is reverse scored. Alpha coefficients of tolerance, absorption, appraisal, regulation, and the whole scale were 0.72, 0.82, 0.78, 0.70, and 0.82, respectively (30). Esmaeilinasab et al. (31) reported a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.67 for this scale. Its validity is assessed using the test-retest method, which produced a correlation coefficient of 0.81 for the whole scale as well as 0.71, 0.69, 0.77, and 0.73 for tolerance, absorption, appraisal, and regulation subscales, respectively.

3.2.3. The Cognitive Flexibility Inventory

It’s developed by Vander Wal and Dennis in 2010 and consists of 20 items. the cognitive flexibility inventory (CFI) is based on a seven-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 7 (“strongly agree”). The total score ranges from 20 to 140. Items 2, 4, 7, 9, 11, and 17 are reverse scored. The CFI has 3 subscales, namely controllability, alternatives, and behavioral justification (32). Its psychometric characteristics are investigated by Shareh et al. in an Iranian sample (33). The test-retest was used to assess the reliability, which produced correlation coefficients of 0.71, 0.72, 0.55, and 0.57 for the whole inventory, controllability, alternatives, and behavioral justification, respectively. In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were 0.90, 0.89, 0.87, and 0.55 for the whole inventory, controllability, alternatives, and behavioral justification, respectively.

3.2.4. The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire

The Cognitive Emotion Regulation questionnaire (CERQ) consists of 18 items and 9 subscales. Responses are given based on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). Dimensions of the CERQ are as follows: putting into perspective, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, acceptance, and refocus on planning are adaptive strategies and self-blame, other-blame, rumination, and catastrophizing. The CERQ should only be used for those aged 12 or higher, whether healthy or sick (34). In Iran, Hasani (35) evaluated its psychometric properties, and Cronbach's alpha coefficients were ranging from 0.68 to 0.82. Therefore, it had a favorable validity.

3.3. Statistical Analyses

Data were analyzed by SPSS using both descriptive (means and standard deviation) and inferential (the Pearson correlation coefficient and stepwise regression analysis) statistics.

4. Results

Demographic information of participants are shown in Table 1. Twenty one (14%) had elementary school degree, 37 (24.7%) middle school degree, 62 (41.3%) high school degree and diploma, and 30 (20%) had a university degree. Concerning the marital status, 16 (10.7%) were single, 112 (74.7%) married, 32 (21.3%) widow, and 2 (1.3%) divorced.

Table 1. The Demographic Information Related to the Subjects Studieda
Demographic VariableFeatureValues
EducationElementary school21 (14)
Middle school37 (24.7)
High school and diploma62 (41.3)
University degrees30 (20)
Marital statusSingle16 (10.7)
Married112 (74.7)
Widow20 (13.3)
Divorced2 (1.3)
OccupationHousewife71 (47.3)
Employee32 (21.3)
Self-employed37 (24.7)
Others10 (6.7)
AgeAge range30 - 60
Mean age42.23
Standard deviation7.97

In terms of occupation, 71 (47.3%) were housewives, 32 (21.3%) had a job, and 37 (24.7%) were self-employed. The youngest and oldest participants were 30 to 60 years, respectively. The mean age of participants was 42.23 years, with a standard deviation of 7.97.

According to Table 2, the Pearson correlation coefficient indicated that emotion regulation strategies had a diverse significant relationship with death anxiety (r = -0.20). Among its adaptive subscales, refocus on planning (r = -0.28), acceptance (r = -0.35), positive reappraisal (r = -0.20), and putting into perspective (r = -0.42) had diverse significant relationships with death anxiety. Moreover, among its maladaptive subscales, catastrophizing (r = 0.19), rumination (r = 0.19), and self-blame (r = 0.16) had direct significant correlations with death anxiety. Other-blame was not significantly associated with death anxiety (r = 0.01). Besides, cognitive flexibility (r = 0.61), in general, and its subscales, i.e., controllability (r = -0.21), alternatives (r = -0.44), and behavioral justification (r = -0.23) had diverse significant relationships with death anxiety. The mean scores and standard deviations are shown in Table 2.

Table 2. The Means, Standard Deviations, and the Pearson Correlation Coefficients of the Variablesa
NumberVariableCorrelation CoefficientValues
Predictor variablesDeath anxiety6.27 ± 1.99
1Emotion regulation strategies-0.20b62.39 ± 6.59
2Refocus on planning-0.28c6.49 ± 1.95
3Acceptance-0.35c6.93 ± 1.94
4Positive reappraisal-0.30c7.22 ± 1.67
5Positive refocusing-0.20b6.92 ± 2
6Putting into perspective-0.42c6.54 ± 1.96
7Catastrophizing0.19b7 ± 1.89
8Rumination0.19b7.1 ± 2.24
9Other-blame0.016.72 ± 2.19
10Self-blame0.16b7.40 ± 1.56
11Cognitive flexibility-0.61c79.34 ± 22.26
12Controllability-0.21c30 ± 7.33
13Alternatives-0.44c47.30 ± 6.92
14Behavioral justification-0.23c9.12 ± 2.82
15Overall distress tolerance-0.21b38.64 ± 6.15
16Tolerance-0.18b7.27 ± 1.41
17Absorption0.52c8 ± 1.74
18Appraisal-0.15b15.56 ± 3.90
19Regulation-0.17b7.62 ± 1.72

As shown in Table 2, distress tolerance (r = -0.21) had a diverse significant relationship with death anxiety. Among its subscales, tolerance (r = -0.18), appraisal (R = -0.15), and regulation (r = -0.17) had diverse significant correlations with death anxiety; however, absorption (r = 0.52) had a direct significant relationship with death anxiety. The stepwise regression analysis was used to examine the role of each predictor variable and its subscales in explaining the variance of the criterion variable (death anxiety). Before conducting this analysis, its assumptions were investigated.

According to Table 3, almost all variables’ skewness and kurtosis scores were in the range of +2 to -2; hence, the data distribution was normal. Furthermore, the tolerance index of the predictor variable and the variance inflation factor were both greater than 0.1 and smaller than 10; therefore, we did not violate the assumption of multicollinearity (28). Accordingly, the regression analysis was performed.

Table 3. The Results of the Normality Tests
VariableSkewnessKurtosisToleranceBased on the VIF
Death anxiety-0.720.42
Emotion regulation strategies-0.590.000.412.40
Refocus on planning-0.62-0.060.961.03
Acceptance-0.46-0.530.891.12
Positive reappraisal-0.590.140.323
Positive refocusing-1-0.170.921.07
Putting into perspective-0.46-0.670.891.12
Catastrophizing-0.60-0.340.671.49
Rumination-1-0.400.961
Other-blame-1-0.540.611.62
Self-blame-0.52-0.030.402.48
Cognitive flexibility3-211
Controllability-0.200.070.871.13
Alternatives-10.000.581.70
Behavioral justification-0.91-0.170.971
Overall score of distress tolerance-0.61-0.290.109.84
Tolerance0.340.480.601.64
Absorption-0.900.310.831.19
Appraisal-0.850.280.631.56
Regulation-0.710.390.861.16

According to Table 4, the results of the stepwise multivariate regression analysis showed that when predicting death anxiety via emotion regulation strategies, cognitive flexibility, and distress tolerance, in the first step, overall cognitive flexibility, rumination, absorption, refocus on planning, positive refocusing, alternatives, catastrophizing, positive reappraisal, and distress tolerance, respectively, entered the equation and could explain 71% of the variance in death anxiety (R2 = 0.71). Based on the significant level of the one-way analysis (F), the regression model was significant at all steps (P < 0.0005). Additionally, checking the variables’ standardized beta coefficients showed that overall cognitive flexibility had the strongest role in explaining death anxiety (beta = -0.67). The significance of the t value (P < 0.0005) a significant contribution.

Table 4. The Results of the Stepwise Regression Analysis to Predict Death Anxiety via the Research Variables
StepVariableRR2Adjusted R2FBBetaTSig
FirstOverall cognitive flexibility0.610.380.3792-0.05-0.61-5.590.0005
SecondRumination0.690.480.4768.850.280.325.340.0005
ThirdAbsorption0.740.550.5461.400.340.294.950.0005
FourthRefocus of planning0.780.610.6058.29-0.25-0.244.710.0005
FifthPositive refocusing0.790.630.6250.62-0.14-0.15-2.870.0005
SixthAlternatives0.810.660.6547.54-0.06-0.22-3.500.001
SeventhCatastrophizing0.820.680.6643.320.150.152.500.01
EighthPositive reappraisal0.830.700.6841.26-0.27-0.238.250.0005
NinthTolerance0.840.710.6939.350.220.162.810.0005

5. Discussion

Based on the findings, CER strategies had significant and diverse associations with death anxiety. Moreover, refocus on planning, acceptance, positive reappraisal, and putting into perspective (adaptive strategies) had diverse significant relationships with death anxiety, catastrophizing, rumination, and self-blame (maladaptive strategies) had significant direct correlations with death anxiety. These findings are consistent results reported by some of the previous studies (6, 7, 36, 37) and inconsistent with others, for example, the study by Zare and Solgi (38). The observed discrepancy can be attributed to factors such as using different sample sizes or characteristics of participants. For example, Zare and Solgi (38) study has investigated university students with lower levels of exposure to stressful events, thoughts, and situations compared to the participants of the present study.

To explain these findings, it can be argued that when confronted with stressful situations, people who use positive strategies experience lower levels of anxiety. In other words, the negative association between adaptive cognitive regulation strategies and death anxiety can be attributed to the persons’ ability to change his/her view in the face of stressful events and paying attention to the positive and long-term outcomes, which in turn decreases the level of stress and helps the person to reframe the events to accept what cannot be controlled, which results in reduced stress. In contrast, those who employ negative strategies in stressful situations experience higher levels of anxiety, through hasty reactions, instead of using appropriate methods. The source of stress can be recognized by applying efficient emotion regulation strategies; that result in increased self-esteem and self-confidence as well as reduced anxiety and distress associated with hypertension (39).

The present study also showed that cognitive flexibility and its subscales had diverse and significant relationships with death anxiety. which is in line with the results of Ghodrati Mirkoohi et al. (40). To explain this finding, it can be argued that those with high anxiety think about the stressful situation as controllable events. Therefore, in the face of stressful events, such as thinking about death, they cannot come up with alternative positive thoughts to replace negative thoughts. Inflexible stylistics exacerbate the anxiety by creating biases in the reception of dysfunctional thoughts (41). In other words, those with flexible thinking can use appropriate justifications and accept challenging situations or stressful events, which in turn, causes experiencing less death anxiety compared to those with low flexibility (42).

Moreover, another important finding was the diverse significant relationship between distress tolerance and death anxiety. Among its subscales, tolerance, appraisal, and regulation had diverse significant relationships with death anxiety; however, absorption had a significant direct correlation. This finding is consistent with other studies (43). To explain this finding, it can be argued that when faced with a stressful situation, such as death, people interpret the negative emotions as if they will continue for a long time, are uncontrollable, will gradually exacerbate, and have no meanings. Besides, they think as the only ones who experience such emotions. On the other hand, they consider such events as intolerable (44). Not only negative emotions are important, but their interpretations and the applied strategies are also influential (45).

The current study had limitations, including only investigating women in the city of Rasht. Therefore, caution should be exercised when generalizing the results to other communities. Furthermore, cultural and ethnic issues also contribute to stressful events and how to deal with them. Also, the data were collected using self-report scales, which are prone to partiality because of unconscious defenses and bias in answering the items. Factors such as education, occupation, lifestyle, and levels of awareness, health information, and mental health literacy also affect the relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies, cognitive flexibility, and distress tolerance, and death anxiety.

5.1. Conclusions

This study revealed the significant impact of cognitive emotion regulation strategies, cognitive flexibility, and distress tolerance on the death anxiety in patients with hypertension. Death has always been an important concern of human beings, and thinking about it causes anxiety and stress, particularly for those with ill-health such as hypertension, which has psychological consequences. Therefore, the findings of the present study can be used by therapists to treat emotional and anxiety disorders. The authors recommend applying the aforementioned strategies to improve CER strategies, cognitive flexibility, and distress tolerance in women with hypertension. Besides, such skills can be transferred to patients through courses and workshops. Also, investigating the research hypothesis in practice in future studies would be useful. Additionally, the findings of the present study are in line with the results of previous research and theories related to these variables and their associations with hypertension. Finally, the authors recommend performing studies on both males and females.

Acknowledgements

References

  • 1.

    Turner JR, Viera AJ, Shimbo D. Ambulatory blood pressure monitoring in clinical practice: a review. Am J Med. 2015;128(1):14-20. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2014.07.021. [PubMed: 25107387]. [PubMed Central: PMC4877527].

  • 2.

    Abniki E, Abolghasemi A, Abbasi M, Moazzez R, Jalali R. The effect of group cognitive-behavioral intervention in stress management on improved hardiness and self-control in depressed women. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015.

  • 3.

    Cassell WA, Dubey BL. Anatomical Rorschach Responses and Death Symbolism. J Projective Psychol Mental Health. 2018;25(2).

  • 4.

    McClatchey IS, King S. The Impact of Death Education on Fear of Death and Death Anxiety Among Human Services Students. Omega (Westport). 2015;71(4):343-61. doi: 10.1177/0030222815572606. [PubMed: 26665963].

  • 5.

    Barnett MD, Anderson EA, Marsden A3. Is death anxiety more closely linked with optimism or pessimism among older adults? Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2018;77:169-73. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2018.05.003. [PubMed: 29783138].

  • 6.

    Razavi V, Amini chokami A. [Relationship between Social Support and Hope and Death Anxiety among the Old People of Tehran Omid Cultural Center]. Int J Life Sci. 2015;9(2):65-70. Persian. doi: 10.3126/ijls.v9i2.12052.

  • 7.

    Gross JJ. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: The Guilford Press; 2013.

  • 8.

    Domaradzka E, Fajkowska M. Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies in Anxiety and Depression Understood as Types of Personality. Front Psychol. 2018;9:856. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00856. [PubMed: 29946277]. [PubMed Central: PMC6005992].

  • 9.

    Ramesh S, Ghazian M, Rafiepoor A, Safari AR. [The Mediator Role of Depression and Anxiety in the Relationship between Cognitive Emotional Regulation and Self-care in Type 2 Diabetes]. Pajouhan Scientific Journal. 2018;16(4):37-45. Persian. doi: 10.21859/psj.16.4.37.

  • 10.

    Farahani S, Rafiepoor A, Jafari T. The Role of Mediator of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies in the Relationship Between Mindfulness and Perceived Stress in People with High Blood Pressure. J Appl Behav Sci. 2020;6(2):52-9.

  • 11.

    Hayes SC. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, Relational Frame Theory, and the Third Wave of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies - Republished Article. Behav Ther. 2016;47(6):869-85. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.11.006. [PubMed: 27993338].

  • 12.

    Moitra E, Gaudiano BA. [A psychological flexibility model of medication adherence in psychotic-spectrum disorders]. J Contextual Behav Sci. 2016;5(4):252-7. Persain. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2016.10.003.

  • 13.

    Wallace DP, McCracken LM, Weiss KE, Harbeck-Weber C. The role of parent psychological flexibility in relation to adolescent chronic pain: further instrument development. J Pain. 2015;16(3):235-46. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2014.11.013. [PubMed: 25499065].

  • 14.

    Dajani DR, Uddin LQ. Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38(9):571-8.

  • 15.

    Basharat MA, GranemayePour SH, Pournqd AA, Horni Z, Habibnejad M, Aghaee Fateh S. [Relationship between alexithymia and interpersonal problems: moderating effect of attachment styles]. Contemp Psychol. 2014;9(1):3-16. Persian.

  • 16.

    Safari Mousavi S, Nadri M, Amiri M, Radffar F, Farokhcheh M. [The predictive role of psychological flexibility and cognitive emotion regulation strategies on depression, anxiety and stress in type 2 diabetic patients]. MEJDS. 2019;50(9):1-6. Persian.

  • 17.

    Bonfils KA, Minor KS, Leonhardt BL, Lysaker PH. Metacognitive self-reflectivity moderates the relationship between distress tolerance and empathy in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2018;265:1-6. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.04.042. [PubMed: 29679792]. [PubMed Central: PMC6309172].

  • 18.

    Reitzel LR, Smith NG, Obasi EM, Forney M, Leventhal AM. Perceived distress tolerance accounts for the covariance between discrimination experiences and anxiety symptoms among sexual minority adults. J Anxiety Disord. 2017;48:22-7. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.07.006. [PubMed: 27475254]. [PubMed Central: PMC5253127].

  • 19.

    Veilleux JC. The relationship between distress tolerance and cigarette smoking: A systematic review and synthesis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2019;71:78-89. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.01.003. [PubMed: 30691959].

  • 20.

    Forouzanfar A. [Distress tolerance: Theory, mechanism and relation to psychopathology]. Rooyesh J. 2017;6(2):239-62. Persian.

  • 21.

    Vujanovic AA, Dutcher CD, Berenz EC. Multimodal examination of distress tolerance and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in acute-care psychiatric inpatients. J Anxiety Disord. 2017;48:45-53. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.08.005. [PubMed: 27605231].

  • 22.

    Laposa JM, Collimore KC, Hawley LL, Rector NA. Distress tolerance in OCD and anxiety disorders, and its relationship with anxiety sensitivity and intolerance of uncertainty. J Anxiety Disord. 2015;33:8-14. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.04.003. [PubMed: 25956557].

  • 23.

    Onyedibe MCC, Ibeagha PN, Onyishi IE. Distress tolerance moderates the relationship between anger experience and elevated blood pressure. S Afr J Psychol. 2019;50(1):39-53. doi: 10.1177/0081246319832540.

  • 24.

    Naomi DL, Fisher GH. Hypertensive vascular disease. New York: Mac Graw Hill; 2004.

  • 25.

    Gall M, Borg W, Gall J. [Quantitative and Qualitative Research Methods in Educational Sciences and Psychology] (Translated by Nasr, Ahmad Reza et al.)]. Tehran, Iran: University Humanities Textbooks; 1996. Persian.

  • 26.

    Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, Ullman JB. Using multivariate statistics. 5. Pearson Boston, MA; 2007.

  • 27.

    World Health Organization. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. B World Health Organ. 2001;79(4):373-4.

  • 28.

    Diedrich A, Hofmann SG, Cuijpers P, Berking M. Self-compassion enhances the efficacy of explicit cognitive reappraisal as an emotion regulation strategy in individuals with major depressive disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2016;82:1-10. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.04.003. [PubMed: 27152671].

  • 29.

    Gholami M, Asghari P, Hoseini Nasrabadi SS, Sadat Z, Saberi F. [The Relationship between Attitude and Practical Commitment to Prayer and Death Anxiety in Patients with Chronic Renal Failure]. Iran J Psychiatr Nurs. 2020;7(6):18-25. Persian.

  • 30.

    Drougari S. [Relationship between trusting god and distress tolerance with quality of life among students of Payam-e-Noor University in Qom]. Qom, Iran: Qom Branch, Islamic Azad University; 2015. Persian.

  • 31.

    Esmaeilinasab M, Andami Khoshk A, Azarmi H, Samar Rakhi A. [The predicting role of difficulties in emotion regulation and distress tolerance in students’ addiction potential]. J Res Addict. 2014;8(29):49-63. Persian.

  • 32.

    Johnco C, Wuthrich VM, Rapee RM. Reliability and validity of two self-report measures of cognitive flexibility. Psychol Assess. 2014;26(4):1381-7. doi: 10.1037/a0038009. [PubMed: 25265414].

  • 33.

    Shareh H, Farmani A, Soltani E. [Investigating the reliability and validity of the Cognitive Flexibility Inventory (CFI-I) among Iranian university students]. Pract Clin Psychol. 2014;2(1):43-50. Persian.

  • 34.

    Besharat MA. [Cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire: instruction and scoring]. Dev Pschol. 2017;130(50):221-3. Persian.

  • 35.

    Hasani J. [The reliability and validity of the short form of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire]. J Res Behav Sci. 2011;9(4):229-40. Persian.

  • 36.

    Huh HJ, Kim KH, Lee HK, Chae JH. The relationship between childhood trauma and the severity of adulthood depression and anxiety symptoms in a clinical sample: The mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. J Affect Disord. 2017;213:44-50. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.02.009. [PubMed: 28189964].

  • 37.

    Mohebi M, Zarei S. [The relationship between emotion regulation strategies and state and trait competitive anxiety in South Korean Ambassador's Cup taekwondo athletes]. Shenakht J Psychol Psychiatr. 2019;6(2):86-101. Persian. doi: 10.29252/shenakht.6.2.86.

  • 38.

    Zare H, Solgi Z. [Investigation of the relationship between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depression, anxiety and stress in students]. J Res Psychol Health. 2012;6(3):19-29. Persian.

  • 39.

    Jafari A, Deghani Arani Z. [Predicting psychological wellbeing of women with hypertension based on Perceived stress with mediating role cognitive emotional regulation]. Iran J Psychiatr Nurs. 2019;7(2):39-47. Persian.

  • 40.

    Ghodrati Mirkoohi M, Waldaran N, Abdollahi Bakhrabadi G. [Prognostic cognitive flexibility of methadone-dependent people based on methadone, anxiety, duration and methadone intake]. Quart J Res Addict. 2017;42:182-95. Persian.

  • 41.

    Rahafar A, Sadeghi JM, Sadeghpour A, Mirzaie SH. [Surveying psychometric features of Persian version of morning-eventide questionnaire]. Clin Psychol Personal. 2013;2(8):109-22. Persian.

  • 42.

    Eskandari H, Pajouhinia S, Abavisan Y. [Explanation of psychological problems based on cognitive flexibility and self differentiation]. Shefaye Khatam. 2016;4(3):18-27. Persian.

  • 43.

    Michel NM, Rowa K, Young L, McCabe RE. Emotional distress tolerance across anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. 2016;40:94-103. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.04.009. [PubMed: 27161839].

  • 44.

    Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A, Vujanovic AA. Distress tolerance: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Guilfod Publication; 2011.

  • 45.

    Mitchell MA, Riccardi CJ, Keough ME, Timpano KR, Schmidt NB. Understanding the associations among anxiety sensitivity, distress tolerance, and discomfort intolerance: a comparison of three models. J Anxiety Disord. 2013;27(1):147-54. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.12.003. [PubMed: 23376602].

Copyright © 2020, Jundishapur Journal of Chronic Disease Care. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits copy and redistribute the material just in noncommercial usages, provided the original work is properly cited.